000 | 03379nam a22003137a 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
999 |
_c8648 _d8648 |
||
005 | 20250625151659.0 | ||
008 | 240501s2020 -nz|| |||| 00| 0 eng d | ||
020 | _a978-0-9951244-5-5 | ||
040 | _aAFVC | ||
100 |
_910902 _aMorton, Susan M. B. |
||
245 |
_aEarly self-control development : _bprevalence, persistence and change in a NZ cohort _cSusan M. B. Morton, Elizabeth R. Peterson, Jude Buckley and Siobhan Murray |
||
260 |
_bMinistry of Social Development, _c2020 |
||
300 | _aelectronic document (58 pages) ; PDF file | ||
520 | _aResearch has shown that higher levels of self-control in childhood are associated with improved health and financial outcomes, life satisfaction and decreased levels of substance abuse and criminal convictions in adulthood (Moffitt et al., 2011). Based on this retrospective analysis, the positive development of self-control is of interest to policy makers looking to promote success across the health, education, economic and social domains in adulthood. Few studies to date have assessed early self-control at a population level; thus, less is known about the emergence of self-control in the early years of life. One exception is The Dunedin Multidisciplinary Health and Development Study which assessed self-control using a single composite self-control measure created from assessments taken between the ages of 3-11 years. They found that lower self-control was related to later poor health and financial outcomes and increased criminal offending in adulthood (Moffitt et al., 2011, 2013). This study uses data from the contemporary longitudinal Growing Up in New Zealand (GUiNZ) study to increase our understanding of self-control development in the first five years of life. The GUiNZ study follows the development of around 6,800 children born in 2009 and 2010. Children were assessed using a variety of self-control related measures when they were 9 months, 2 and 4.5 years of age. Our primary aims were to: 1. Devise indices of self-control using relevant measures of children’s behaviour at 9 months, 2 years and 4.5 years of age. 2. Validate the indices of self-control against the internationally recognised Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ). 3. Identify the early childhood familial and situational factors that promote or undermine the development of self-control. 4. Describe the stability of pre-schoolers' self-control and explore if there is an age where children at greater risk can be identified. 5. Identify factors that distinguish children with low self-control from those without low self-control across the preschool period. (From the Executive summary). Record #8648 | ||
650 | 4 |
_92691 _aBRAIN DEVELOPMENT |
|
650 | 4 |
_aCHILD DEVELOPMENT _9109 |
|
650 | 0 |
_98305 _aGrowing Up in New Zealand study (GUiNZ) |
|
650 | 4 |
_aINFANTS _9313 |
|
650 | 4 |
_aINTERVENTION _9326 |
|
650 | 4 |
_aLONGITUDINAL STUDIES _9351 |
|
650 | 4 |
_aPARENTING _9429 |
|
650 | 4 |
_aRISK FACTORS _9505 |
|
651 | 4 |
_aNEW ZEALAND _92588 |
|
700 |
_95629 _aPeterson, Elizabeth R. |
||
700 |
_aBuckley, Jude _912879 |
||
700 |
_aMurray, Siobhan _912880 |
||
856 |
_uhttps://www.msd.govt.nz/documents/about-msd-and-our-work/publications-resources/research/early-self-control-development/early-self-control-development.pdf _zDownload report, PDF |
||
942 |
_2ddc _cREPORT _hnews127 |