000 04255nab a22003137a 4500
999 _c6795
_d6795
005 20250625151534.0
008 200820s2020 ||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d
040 _aAFVC
100 _aPichler, Anne S.
_99337
245 _aInconsistencies in complainant's accounts of child sexual abuse arising in their cross-examination
_cAnne Sophie Pichler, Stefanie J. Sharman, Sarah Zydervelt, Nina Westera and Jane Goodman-Delahunty
260 _bTaylor & Francis,
_c2020
500 _aPsychology, Crime & Law, 2020, Advance publication online, 11 August 2020
520 _aA key cross-examination tactic in trials of child sexual abuse (CSA) is to highlight inconsistencies between sources of information to discredit the complainant's account. The present study examined the prevalence, origin and nature of inconsistencies arising in the cross-examination of complainants in CSA trials. Further, we examined the association between these inconsistencies and the types of question that elicited them in the earlier police interview of the child witness (i.e. open-ended, specific, or leading). Transcripts of video recorded interviews (evidence-in-chief) of 73 complainants (15 males, 58 females) and subsequent cross-examinations at trial were coded. Results showed that inconsistencies were raised in the cross-examination of 94.5% of complainants; most between what the children said in their police interview versus their cross-examination. A greater proportion of inconsistencies was associated with specific than open-ended questions asked in the police interview. However, open-ended questions were associated with some inconsistencies, perhaps due to the longer answers they elicited. Shorter police interviews relying mainly on open-ended questions may minimise the opportunity for inconsistencies to arise in cross-examinations. Judges and juries require education about inconsistencies that arise from memory's reconstructive nature and lay people's tendency to use these inconsistencies to make inferences about the unreliability of witnesses. (Authors' abstract). Record #6795
526 _aA key cross-examination tactic in trials of child sexual abuse (CSA) is to highlight inconsistencies between sources of information to discredit the complainant's account. The present study examined the prevalence, origin and nature of inconsistencies arising in the cross-examination of complainants in CSA trials. Further, we examined the association between these inconsistencies and the types of question that elicited them in the earlier police interview of the child witness (i.e. open-ended, specific, or leading). Transcripts of videorecorded interviews (evidence-in-chief) of 73 complainants (15 males, 58 females) and subsequent cross-examinations at trial were coded. Results showed that inconsistencies were raised in the cross-examination of 94.5% of complainants; most between what the children said in their police interview versus their cross-examination. A greater proportion of inconsistencies was associated with specific than open-ended questions asked in the police interview. However, open-ended questions were associated with some inconsistencies, perhaps due to the longer answers they elicited. Shorter police interviews relying mainly on open-ended questions may minimise the opportunity for inconsistencies to arise in cross-examinations. Judges and juries require education about inconsistencies that arise from memory's reconstructive nature and lay people's tendency to use these inconsistencies to make inferences about the unreliability of witnesses. (Authors' abstract). Record #6795
650 _aCHILD SEXUAL ABUSE
_9121
650 _aCRIMINAL JUSTICE
_9167
650 _aEVIDENCE
_9237
650 4 _9445
_aPOLICE PROCEDURES
651 _aINTERNATIONAL
_93624
651 4 _aAUSTRALIA
_92597
700 _aSharman, Stefanie J.
_94939
700 _aZydervelt, Sarah
_99338
700 _aWestera, Nina
_99339
700 _aGoodman-Delahunty, Jane
_93818
773 0 _tPsychology, Crime & Law, 2020, Advance publication online, 11 August 2020
830 _aPsychology, Crime & Law
_98609
856 _uhttps://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2020.1805743
_zDOI: 10.1080/1068316X.2020.1805743
942 _2ddc
_cARTICLE