Inconsistencies in complainant's accounts of child sexual abuse arising in their cross-examination (Record no. 6795)
[ view plain ]
000 -LEADER | |
---|---|
fixed length control field | 04255nab a22003137a 4500 |
005 - DATE AND TIME OF LATEST TRANSACTION | |
control field | 20250625151534.0 |
008 - FIXED-LENGTH DATA ELEMENTS--GENERAL INFORMATION | |
fixed length control field | 200820s2020 ||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d |
040 ## - CATALOGING SOURCE | |
Original cataloging agency | AFVC |
100 ## - MAIN ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME | |
Personal name | Pichler, Anne S. |
9 (RLIN) | 9337 |
245 ## - TITLE STATEMENT | |
Title | Inconsistencies in complainant's accounts of child sexual abuse arising in their cross-examination |
Statement of responsibility, etc | Anne Sophie Pichler, Stefanie J. Sharman, Sarah Zydervelt, Nina Westera and Jane Goodman-Delahunty |
260 ## - PUBLICATION, DISTRIBUTION, ETC. (IMPRINT) | |
Name of publisher, distributor, etc | Taylor & Francis, |
Date of publication, distribution, etc | 2020 |
500 ## - GENERAL NOTE | |
General note | Psychology, Crime & Law, 2020, Advance publication online, 11 August 2020 |
520 ## - SUMMARY, ETC. | |
Summary, etc | A key cross-examination tactic in trials of child sexual abuse (CSA) is to highlight inconsistencies between sources of information to discredit the complainant's account. The present study examined the prevalence, origin and nature of inconsistencies arising in the cross-examination of complainants in CSA trials. Further, we examined the association between these inconsistencies and the types of question that elicited them in the earlier police interview of the child witness (i.e. open-ended, specific, or leading). Transcripts of video recorded interviews (evidence-in-chief) of 73 complainants (15 males, 58 females) and subsequent cross-examinations at trial were coded. Results showed that inconsistencies were raised in the cross-examination of 94.5% of complainants; most between what the children said in their police interview versus their cross-examination. A greater proportion of inconsistencies was associated with specific than open-ended questions asked in the police interview. However, open-ended questions were associated with some inconsistencies, perhaps due to the longer answers they elicited. Shorter police interviews relying mainly on open-ended questions may minimise the opportunity for inconsistencies to arise in cross-examinations. Judges and juries require education about inconsistencies that arise from memory's reconstructive nature and lay people's tendency to use these inconsistencies to make inferences about the unreliability of witnesses. (Authors' abstract). Record #6795 |
526 ## - STUDY PROGRAM INFORMATION NOTE | |
Program name | A key cross-examination tactic in trials of child sexual abuse (CSA) is to highlight inconsistencies between sources of information to discredit the complainant's account. The present study examined the prevalence, origin and nature of inconsistencies arising in the cross-examination of complainants in CSA trials. Further, we examined the association between these inconsistencies and the types of question that elicited them in the earlier police interview of the child witness (i.e. open-ended, specific, or leading). Transcripts of videorecorded interviews (evidence-in-chief) of 73 complainants (15 males, 58 females) and subsequent cross-examinations at trial were coded. Results showed that inconsistencies were raised in the cross-examination of 94.5% of complainants; most between what the children said in their police interview versus their cross-examination. A greater proportion of inconsistencies was associated with specific than open-ended questions asked in the police interview. However, open-ended questions were associated with some inconsistencies, perhaps due to the longer answers they elicited. Shorter police interviews relying mainly on open-ended questions may minimise the opportunity for inconsistencies to arise in cross-examinations. Judges and juries require education about inconsistencies that arise from memory's reconstructive nature and lay people's tendency to use these inconsistencies to make inferences about the unreliability of witnesses. (Authors' abstract). Record #6795 |
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM | |
Topical term or geographic name as entry element | CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE |
9 (RLIN) | 121 |
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM | |
Topical term or geographic name as entry element | CRIMINAL JUSTICE |
9 (RLIN) | 167 |
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM | |
Topical term or geographic name as entry element | EVIDENCE |
9 (RLIN) | 237 |
650 #4 - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM | |
9 (RLIN) | 445 |
Topical term or geographic name as entry element | POLICE PROCEDURES |
651 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--GEOGRAPHIC NAME | |
Geographic name | INTERNATIONAL |
9 (RLIN) | 3624 |
651 #4 - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--GEOGRAPHIC NAME | |
Geographic name | AUSTRALIA |
9 (RLIN) | 2597 |
700 ## - ADDED ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME | |
Personal name | Sharman, Stefanie J. |
9 (RLIN) | 4939 |
700 ## - ADDED ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME | |
Personal name | Zydervelt, Sarah |
9 (RLIN) | 9338 |
700 ## - ADDED ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME | |
Personal name | Westera, Nina |
9 (RLIN) | 9339 |
700 ## - ADDED ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME | |
Personal name | Goodman-Delahunty, Jane |
9 (RLIN) | 3818 |
773 0# - HOST ITEM ENTRY | |
Title | Psychology, Crime & Law, 2020, Advance publication online, 11 August 2020 |
830 ## - SERIES ADDED ENTRY--UNIFORM TITLE | |
Uniform title | Psychology, Crime & Law |
9 (RLIN) | 8609 |
856 ## - ELECTRONIC LOCATION AND ACCESS | |
Uniform Resource Identifier | <a href="https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2020.1805743">https://doi.org/10.1080/1068316X.2020.1805743</a> |
Public note | DOI: 10.1080/1068316X.2020.1805743 |
942 ## - ADDED ENTRY ELEMENTS (KOHA) | |
Source of classification or shelving scheme | Dewey Decimal Classification |
Koha item type | Journal article |
No items available.