Children’s allegations of sexual abuse in criminal trials : (Record no. 6692)

MARC details
000 -LEADER
fixed length control field 05247nam a22003017a 4500
005 - DATE AND TIME OF LATEST TRANSACTION
control field 20250625151529.0
008 - FIXED-LENGTH DATA ELEMENTS--GENERAL INFORMATION
fixed length control field 200612s2020 ||||| |||| 00| 0 eng d
040 ## - CATALOGING SOURCE
Original cataloging agency AFVC
100 ## - MAIN ENTRY--PERSONAL NAME
Personal name Stolzenberg, Stacia N.
9 (RLIN) 9179
245 ## - TITLE STATEMENT
Title Children’s allegations of sexual abuse in criminal trials :
Remainder of title assessing defense attacks on credibility and identifying effective prosecution methods
Statement of responsibility, etc Stacia N. Stolzenberg
260 ## - PUBLICATION, DISTRIBUTION, ETC. (IMPRINT)
Name of publisher, distributor, etc NCJRS,
Date of publication, distribution, etc 2020
300 ## - PHYSICAL DESCRIPTION
Extent electronic document (35 pages) ; PDF file
500 ## - GENERAL NOTE
General note Available April 2020
500 ## - GENERAL NOTE
General note This resource has not been published by the U.S. Department of<br/>Justice. This resource is being made publically available through the Office of Justice Programs’ National Criminal Justice Reference Service. Document number: 254627
520 ## - SUMMARY, ETC.
Summary, etc Due to delays in reporting, lacking witnesses, and infrequent medical and physical<br/>evidence, in criminal investigations of alleged child sexual abuse (CSA), children’s reports of abuse become central to determining whether a crime occurred. While researchers acknowledge that developmental vulnerabilities make children particularly susceptible to courtroom questioning, potentially influencing the reliability and validity of their in-court reports, little attention has been paid to how children are questioned in-court. Only one previous dataset, collected on older cases, can speak to questioning practices in the United States. This was the purpose of the present project: to examine how attorneys establish and attack children’s credibility. In addition, we were interested in assessing how attorneys would phrase questions,<br/>how children would respond, and whether questioning practices would exhibit developmental sensitivity.<br/><br/>Collecting cases prosecuted between 2005 and 2015 in Maricopa County, Arizona, we examined transcripts of 134 minors (5-17-year-olds) testifying about alleged child sexual abuse in criminal trials. The majority of cases involved allegations against a familiar, if not familial, adult. Children commonly alleged repeated abuse. All question and answer pairs were coded for whether questions assessed three areas of credibility: suggestibility and honesty, plausibility, and consistency. In addition, all question and answer pairs were systematically coded for the linguistic form of each attorney question and each child’s subsequent response.<br/><br/>Consistency concerns seemed to be embedded in nearly all questions in some capacity, representing 79% of prosecutor questions and 89% of defense questions. In addition, prosecutors devoted more time to establishing plausibility than did the defense, at the expense of addressing suggestibility and honesty, to which the defense gave proportionally more attention. Prosecutors also asked the fewest credibility questions of the youngest children, while defense attorneys did<br/>not differ in the proportion of credibility questions by the age of the child. Closed-ended questions accounted for three-out-of-four attorney questions; while children provided elaborative responses to open questions, they provided unleaborative responses to closed-ended questions. We found that declarative questions, or statements posed as questions (e.g. “He hit you?”) were common, representing 21% of overall questions, and indirect yes/no questions (e.g. “Do you remember if he hit you?”) were common, describing 11% of all questions asked. Children<br/>provided the most unelaborative and fewest elaborative responses to declarative questions, and the most non-substantive responses to indirect yes/no questions, when compared to other closedended questions. While prosecutors were more likely to ask open-questions, and less likely to ask suggestive questions, when compared to the defense, they had similar rates of closed-ended questions (including declarative and indirect yes/no questions). There were few differences in<br/>questioning practices, or response patterns, based on the age of the child.<br/><br/>Our data demonstrate that declarative and indirect yes/no questions produce problematic response patterns. In addition, the issues of consistency, or inconsistency, dominate in courtroom investigations of CSA, whereas issues of suggestive influence, honesty, and plausibility receive significantly less attention. Furthermore, according to the kinds of credibility questions observed,<br/>children may not be developmentally mature enough to answer the questions asked of them - a tactic that may in itself be undermining credibility, particularly by the defense. Researchers should work alongside prosecuting attorneys to develop effective training methods, as little is known about how attorneys learn how to question children in these cases. (Author's abstract). Record #6692
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE
9 (RLIN) 121
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element CHILDREN
9 (RLIN) 127
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element COURTS
9 (RLIN) 162
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element CRIMINAL JUSTICE
9 (RLIN) 167
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element DISCLOSURE
9 (RLIN) 199
650 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element EVIDENCE
9 (RLIN) 237
650 #0 - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element VICTIMS OF SEXUAL VIOLENCE
9 (RLIN) 6716
650 #0 - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--TOPICAL TERM
Topical term or geographic name as entry element YOUNG PEOPLE
9 (RLIN) 660
651 ## - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--GEOGRAPHIC NAME
Geographic name INTERNATIONAL
9 (RLIN) 3624
651 #4 - SUBJECT ADDED ENTRY--GEOGRAPHIC NAME
Geographic name UNITED STATES
9 (RLIN) 2646
856 ## - ELECTRONIC LOCATION AND ACCESS
Uniform Resource Identifier <a href="https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/254627.pdf?utm_content=default&utm_medium=email&utm_source=govdelivery">https://www.ncjrs.gov/pdffiles1/nij/grants/254627.pdf?utm_content=default&utm_medium=email&utm_source=govdelivery</a>
942 ## - ADDED ENTRY ELEMENTS (KOHA)
Source of classification or shelving scheme Dewey Decimal Classification
Koha item type Report
Holdings
Withdrawn status Lost status Source of classification or shelving scheme Damaged status Not for loan Home library Current library Date acquired Total Checkouts Full call number Barcode Date last seen Price effective from Koha item type
    Dewey Decimal Classification     Vine library Vine library 12/06/2020   Online ON20060018 12/06/2020 12/06/2020 Access online